Paper-to-Podcast

Paper Summary

Title: Problems with Cosine as a Measure of Embedding Similarity for High Frequency Words


Source: arXiv


Authors: Kaitlyn Zhou et al.


Published Date: 2022-05-10

Podcast Transcript

Hello, and welcome to paper-to-podcast. Today, we're diving into the fascinating world of language models and, in particular, a study titled "Problems with Cosine as a Measure of Embedding Similarity for High Frequency Words," by Kaitlyn Zhou and colleagues.

Now, if you're like me, you're probably thinking, "'Cosine', 'embedding', 'high frequency words'...what on earth are you talking about?" Well, don't worry, folks. We're going to break it all down for you – and have a few laughs along the way.

The crux of this research is a surprising discovery about how language models handle word similarities. The more often a word shows up in training data – like a pop star who just won't quit – the less similar language models like BERT consider it to other instances of the same word or other words in different contexts.

This is like if you met your doppelganger at a party, but the more you hung out, the less you thought you looked alike. And the researchers found this to be true even when controlling for factors like polysemy – words that have multiple meanings – and part-of-speech.

The researchers put the spotlight on cosine similarity, a metric that's like the thermometer of word similarity in these models. It seems this metric is doing a bit of an underestimation dance when it comes to high-frequency words, as compared to human judgments.

In one study, with all the gravity of a high school math test, they found a significant negative association between cosine and frequency among examples with the "same meaning" and "different meaning". They think this discrepancy is due to differences in the "representational geometry" of high and low-frequency words, which, in layman's terms, means that the way these words are represented in the model isn't a straight line and changes based on how often they appear in the training data.

Now, before you start thinking this is all theoretical mumbo jumbo, let me assure you: Zhou and colleagues have put in the hard yards. They used well-established datasets and controlled for other factors to ensure the robustness of their results. Their use of ordinary least squares regression – yes, it's a thing – adds statistical rigor to their analysis.

However, like any good scientific endeavor, the study is not without limitations. The findings might not necessarily apply to other types of embeddings or similarity metrics. Also, their research does not explore potential mitigation strategies for the identified frequency-related distortions in detail. The lack of examination on how these distortions could affect downstream applications, like language translation or sentiment analysis, is another limitation.

But hey, every cloud has a silver lining! Understanding these distortions can help improve natural language processing tasks. This research can be applied to refine and enhance the accuracy of tasks such as question answering, information retrieval, and machine translation. Moreover, these findings can influence the development of more fair and unbiased artificial intelligence models.

So, there you have it, folks. A deep dive into the world of cosine similarity and high-frequency words. If you're still awake, congratulations! You're one step closer to becoming a language model expert. And if you're asleep, well, we hope we've given you some interesting dream fodder.

You can find this paper and more on the paper2podcast.com website. Until next time, keep questioning, keep learning, and remember: not all high-frequency words are created equal.

Supporting Analysis

Findings:
The research made some unexpected discoveries about how language models handle word similarities. It turns out that the more often a word shows up in training data, the less similar language models like BERT consider it to other instances of the same word or other words in different contexts. This is despite controlling for factors like polysemy (words with multiple meanings) and part-of-speech. The researchers found that cosine similarity, a common metric for gauging word similarity in these models, underestimates similarity for high-frequency words compared to human judgments. In one study, a regression predicting cosine similarity showed a significant negative association between cosine and frequency among examples with the "same meaning" (R2: 0.13, coeff’s p<0.001) and "different meaning" (R2:0.14, coeff’s p<0.001). They suspect this discrepancy is due to differences in the "representational geometry" of high and low-frequency words. In other words, the way these words are represented in the model isn't linear and changes based on how often they appear in the training data.
Methods:
The researchers in this study used a technique called cosine similarity, which is often used to measure the semantic similarity between two words in many natural language processing tasks. They applied this technique to BERT embeddings, which are essentially the mathematical representations of words in a high dimensional space. The study used two datasets: Word-In-Context (WiC) and Stanford Contextualized Word Similarity dataset (SCWS). WiC contains pairs of words in context, labeled as having the same or different meaning. SCWS, on the other hand, contains crowd judgements of the similarity of two words in context. Using these datasets, the researchers performed a series of regression studies to investigate the relationship between word frequency and cosine similarity. They controlled for factors like part-of-speech and polysemy (the number of meanings a word can have). The goal was to see how word frequency influences the semantic similarity of high-frequency words.
Strengths:
The researchers have meticulously explored a significant problem in Natural Language Processing (NLP) tasks, specifically the underestimation of cosine similarity in high-frequency words, which is a compelling aspect of the research. They have used well-established datasets, such as Word-In-Context (WiC) and Stanford Contextualized Word Similarity dataset (SCWS), which provide reliable data for the research. They adhered to best practices by controlling for other factors such as polysemy, part-of-speech, and lemma, ensuring the robustness of their results. Their use of ordinary least squares regression to measure the effect of word frequency on the cosine similarity of BERT embeddings adds statistical rigor to their analysis. Additionally, the researchers have been transparent about their methodology, using open-source tools and providing their code for others to replicate their work. Their research has potential implications for improving current NLP tasks and metrics, making it a significant contribution to the field. Furthermore, their conjecture about the representational geometry of high and low-frequency words opens up avenues for future research.
Limitations:
The study focuses on the cosine similarity metric within BERT embeddings, so the findings might not necessarily apply to other types of embeddings or similarity metrics. The research also heavily relies on the Word-In-Context (WiC) and Stanford Contextualized Word Similarity (SCWS) datasets, which might not fully capture the complexity and diversity of language use in real-world scenarios. Furthermore, the research does not explore potential mitigation strategies for the identified frequency-related distortions in detail. There is also a lack of examination on how these distortions could potentially affect downstream applications, such as language translation or sentiment analysis. It is also worth noting that this research does not consider the effects of other potential confounding factors, such as word length or syntactic complexity. Finally, the authors acknowledge that their conjectures about representational geometry need to be further explored and validated.
Applications:
Understanding the distortions that high-frequency words have on cosine similarity in BERT embeddings can help improve natural language processing (NLP) tasks. This research can be applied to refine and enhance the accuracy of NLP tasks such as question answering (QA), information retrieval (IR), machine translation (MT), and other tasks that rely on measuring semantic similarity. Moreover, these findings can influence the development of more fair and unbiased artificial intelligence models by shedding light on how training data frequencies can lead to discrepancies in the representation of different subjects. This could be crucial in avoiding the perpetuation of historic power and wealth inequalities. Being aware of the potential inequalities in datasets and the models trained on them can lead to the creation of more transparent and accountable machine learning models.